Skip to main content

Overall intact cognitive function in male X-linked adrenoleukodystrophy adults with normal MRI

Abstract

Background

Men with the hereditary peroxisomal disorder X-linked adrenoleukodystrophy (ALD) are at risk of developing inflammatory demyelinating lesions in the brain. In the absence of inflammatory (post-contrast enhancing) lesions on MRI cognitive function is considered spared, but some form of cognitive dysfunction may nevertheless be present. The aim of this cross-sectional study was to characterize cognitive functioning of ALD men with no or minimal MRI abnormalities, which will define cognitive functioning in this category of patients.

Methods

A neuropsychological battery covering a broad range of cognitive domains, including language, verbal and non-verbal memory, visuoconstruction, executive functioning, and psychomotor speed, was used. Means and proportions of borderline and impaired T scores ≤36 were compared to the standardized norm group and a qualitative case-by-case analysis was performed for participants with T scores ≤36 within ≥2 domains. Patients with MRI abnormalities that were extensive (Loes score > 3) or showed enhancement post-contrast were excluded.

Results

Thirty-three men participated (median age 44 years, range 19–71). Mean performance on verbal fluency was poorer in patients (45.70 ± 8.85 patients vs. 50 ± 10 standardized norm group, p = 0.009), as was the percentage of borderline and impaired scores on visuoconstruction (Beery VMI: 19% patients vs. 8% standardized norm group, p = 0.02; RCFT copy: 81% patients vs. 2% standardized norm group, p < 0.0005) and mental reaction time during a complex decision task (18% patients vs. 8% standardized norm group, p = 0.055). Moreover, 9/33 (27.3%) patients had T scores ≤36 within ≥2 domains.

Conclusions

Given the heterogeneous pattern of mostly borderline scores cognitive functioning seems not impaired in the vast majority of adult ALD males with no or minimal MRI abnormalities. However, borderline to impaired cognitive dysfunction was present in 27.3%, with the majority being borderline scores. Longitudinal studies will have to determine if this reflects early cerebral disease under the detection limit of MRI.

Introduction

Boys and men with the hereditary peroxisomal disorder X-linked adrenoleukodystrophy (ALD) are at risk of developing inflammatory demyelinating lesions in the brain (‘cerebral ALD’) [1]. Although all patients have an ABCD1 mutation, only some develop inflammatory brain lesions and predicting who is not possible. Untreated the brain lesions are usually rapidly progressive and cause severe disability and death. Haematopoietic stem cell transplantation stabilizes lesions if performed in an early stage of the disease [2,3,4,5]. Although overall cognitive functioning is considered spared as long as there are no inflammatory lesions on MRI [6], some form of cognitive dysfunction may be present in patients without lesions [7]. Indeed, in ALD boys with no or minimal MRI abnormalities overall cognitive functioning was intact, but some dysfunction in visual perceptual, visuomotor or visual reasoning skills and verbal skills was present [8,9,10]. Similarly, in 52 adult ALD men with no or minimal MRI abnormalities verbal and visual memory, psychomotor speed, and visuoconstruction were impaired in some of them, however these findings were based on a cognitive test battery that didn’t fully cover all cognitive functions and have not been confirmed in later studies [7]. The detected cognitive dysfunction could reflect functional abnormalities of the white matter caused by the underlying genetic defect or perhaps even very early signs of inflammatory demyelinating lesions under the detection limit of structural MRI.

The purpose of this cross-sectional study was to characterize cognitive functioning of male ALD adults with no or minimal MRI abnormalities. This will define cognitive functioning in this category of ALD patients and provide directives on the neuropsychological requirements of ALD patients during the course of the disease.

Methods

Participants

In this cross-sectional study Dutch ALD patients from the ongoing prospective natural history study (‘The Dutch ALD cohort’) [11] were approached to participate between June 2016 and February 2017. Men aged 18 years or older with available (3.0 Tesla) MRI results were eligible for inclusion. Men with co-morbidity that would interfere with the interpretation of neuropsychological testing results or with MRI abnormalities that were extensive or showed enhancement post-contrast were excluded from participation. MRI abnormalities were considered extensive if the Loes score was over three. The Loes score is an ALD MRI score, which rates the severity of white matter lesions and ranges from 0 (normal) – 34 (abnormal) [12]. MRI’s were scored by two independent physicians (IH and MS). The physicians were blinded to the neuropsychological test results. If the MRI scores varied, they were debated until a consensus was reached. All white matter abnormalities were scored, unless they were small, round and highly aspecific. Atrophy was solely scored in the presence of white matter abnormalities. White matter abnormalities were further categorized into three categories based on their distribution and shape: ALD lesions, vascular lesions and other lesions. Confluent white matter lesions with increased signal intensity on T2-weighted and FLAIR images were considered ALD lesions, whereas diffuse irregular white matter lesions with punctate foci were considered vascular lesions. Lesions that did not appear ALD like or vascular were labelled as other lesions.

Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents

The study protocol was approved by the local Institutional Review Board (METC 2016_012). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Procedure

Participation included one comprehensive neuropsychological assessment and took place at the Amsterdam UMC in Amsterdam, The Netherlands. A standardized neuropsychological battery was composed to examine cognitive (dys)function across different cognitive domains. Test results were compared to Dutch standardized norm groups (N = 276–1600), that corrects for age, education level and/or gender. The neuropsychological tests as well as the Dutch standardized norm groups are frequently used in neuropsychological practice and research (Table 1). The duration of the neuropsychological assessment was approximately two hours and was administered by a well-trained neuropsychologist (in training) in a single session.

Table 1 Neuropsychological assessment battery

Statistical analyses

The data was analysed with IBM SPSS statistics version 24 (IBM Inc.) and MedCalc Statistical Software version 17.9 [24]. Raw scores were converted to standardized T scores. T scores are commonly used for neuropsychological normative data and are comparable with z scores. Like z scores, T scores are a standard score, which are calculated with standardized norm group corrected for age, gender and/or educational level. T scores have a mean of 50 and a standard deviation (SD) of 10, whereas z scores have a mean of 0 and a SD of 1 (e.g., z = − 1 equals T = 40; z = 1 equals, T = 60). Higher T scores represent better cognitive function. For this exploratory study, we considered T scores between 30 and 36 (− 2 SD to − 1.5 SD below standardized norm group mean; ‘borderline’) to indicate borderline scores and T scores of 29 and lower (> − 2 SD below standardized norm group mean; ‘extremely low scores’) as impairment [25, 26]. An exception was the RCFT-copy subtest. On this test the maximum T score is 40. Therefore, RCFT-copy T scores were categorized into ‘Normal’ (T scores ranging from 38 to 40), ‘borderline’ (T scores ranging from 30 to 37) and ‘impaired’ (T scores ≤29) [18, 27].

The Shapiro-Wilk test was used to test the assumption of normality. The distribution of the educational level of the participants, based on the Dutch educational classification scale of Verhage (1983) [28], was compared to the male Dutch population with a chi-square goodness-of-fit test [29]. For comparisons of the neuropsychological test scores between groups first one-sample t tests (normally distributed continuous data) or one-sample Wilcoxon signed rank tests (non-normally distributed continuous data) were used to compare the average T scores with the average T score of the standardized norm group (50 ± 10). Then, one-proportion z-tests were used to compare the proportion of borderline (T scores between 30 and 36) to impaired scores (T scores ≤29) on neuropsychological tests within our cohort with the proportion borderline and impaired scores that occur in the standardized norm group (8%) [25]. Lastly, the distribution of the categorical RCFT copy subtest scores was analysed with chi-square goodness-of-fit tests. The expected frequency was set to a normal distribution in the population. P values < 0.05 (two-tailed) were defined as statistically significant.

Borderline scores in itself are no indication of impairment unless there is a clear decrease over time and the pattern of borderline scores is consistent. Impaired scores reflect impairment [26].

To evaluate the possible effect of minor MRI abnormalities the comparisons of neuropsychological test scores with the standardized norm group were performed four times. First, including all patients (primary analysis). Second, including only patients with a completely normal MRI (subgroup analysis 1). Third, including patients with a completely normal MRI and patients with ALD lesions (Loes score ≤ 3) (subgroup analysis 2) and fourth, including patients with a completely normal MRI and patients with vascular lesions (subgroup analysis 3).

Last, a qualitative case-by-case analysis was performed to see which patients had borderline or impaired scores (T scores ≤36) within 2 or more cognitive domains. Univariate logistic regression analyses were used to evaluate the effect of age or the presence of MRI abnormalities on the qualitative case-by-case analysis outcome (T scores ≤36 within 2 or more cognitive domains yes/no).

Results

Demographics

Of the 39 adult men with ALD participating in the natural history study, 4 patients had a Loes score > 3 and one patient had non-ALD related intellectual disability. The remaining 34 eligible patients were approached for participation, of whom 33 agreed. Certain test battery elements were excluded per participant due to poor eyesight (< 20%) and colour-blindness (TMT, Stroop, Beery, RCFT-copy) in one case; solely colour-blindness (Stroop II, III, III/II) in one case; an essential tremor (TMT A, TMT B, Beery VMI, RCFT-copy, VTS) and daily benzodiazepine use (VTS) in 2 cases; and, inconsistent, extreme negative or unreliable self-report scores on the BRIEF-A in 3 cases. Median age was 44 years (range 19–71). The most frequent education levels were secondary vocational education (14/33) and (higher) secondary education or university of applied sciences (14/33). The distribution of educational levels was significantly different in comparison to the male Dutch population (x2 (4) = 11.806, p = 0.019). The proportion of patients with secondary vocational education and higher secondary education was higher than in the Dutch population, and the proportion of primary or lower vocational education and university bachelor or masters degree was lower (Table 2). White matter lesions on MRI were present in 18/33 (54.5%) patients, including ALD lesions (n = 4), vascular lesions (n = 12) and other types of lesions (n = 2). The other lesions included a lesion suggestive of an old cerebral contusion in one patient and aspecific white matter lesions in another (Table 2). In the patients with vascular lesions the maximum Fazekas grade was one [30].

Table 2 Patient characteristics

Cognitive functioning - primary analyses (including all patients)

First average T scores were compared to standardized norm group values. The mean T score for the letter fluency test (45.70 ± 8.85) was statistically significant lower in patients with a difference of 4.30 (95% confidence interval (CI), − 7.44 to – 1.16), t (32)= − 2.793, p = 0.009). The group means and medians of all other tests with continuous measures were not significantly lower from the mean of the standardized norm group (Table 3). Second, percentages of borderline and impaired T scores (≤36) were compared with the percentage in the standardized norm group (8%) (Table 4). The percentage borderline and impaired T scores on the Beery VMI in the patients (19%) was significantly higher than in the standardized norm group (z = 2.33, p = 0.02). The percentage borderline and impaired T scores on the VTS S3 RT (18%) tended to be higher than in standardized norm group (z = 1.92, p = 0.055). Last, for the RCFT copy subtest results were normal in 2/31, suboptimal in 4/31 and impaired in 25/31. Scores were not distributed as expected (x2 (1)=803.107, p < 0.0005).

Table 3 T scores of adult male ALD patients compared to the standardized norm group (mean = 50)
Table 4 Frequencies of T scores and borderline and impaired T scores (≤ 36) from adult male ALD patients compared to the percentage in the standardized norm group (8%)

Cognitive functioning - subgroup analyses

Besides comparing test scores of all patients with the standardized norm group, three subgroup analyses were performed to evaluate the possible effect of minor MRI abnormalities. Again, average T scores were compared to standardized norm group values. For all subgroup analyses no additional significantly lower mean T scores were detected.

In addition, percentages of borderline and impaired T scores (≤36) were compared with the percentage in the standardized norm group (8%). When solely including subgroup 1 (patients with a completely normal MRI) the percentage borderline and impaired T scores on the VTS-S3-RT became significantly higher than in the standardized norm group (p = 0.045). When including subgroup 2 (patients with a completely normal MRI and patients with ALD lesions with a Loes score ≤ 3) the percentage borderline and impaired T scores on the letter fluency test became significantly higher in comparison to the standardized norm group (p = 0.0032). When including subgroup 3 (patients with a completely normal MRI and patients with minor vascular lesions) the percentage borderline and impaired T scores on the VTS-S3-RT became significantly higher than the standardized norm group (p = 0.021).

For the RCFT copy test subgroup analyses were not possible due to insufficient numbers per category.

Case-by-case analysis

Case-by-case analyses revealed that 6/33 (18.2%) patients had borderline to impaired T scores (T scores ≤36) across 2 cognitive domains and 3/33 (9%) patients had borderline to impaired scores across 3 cognitive domains. Of these 9 patients showing deficits in 2 or more cognitive domains, 5 had a completely normal MRI, 2 had ALD lesions, and 2 had vascular lesions. Of the 6 patients with 2 affected cognitive domains, psychomotor speed was most prevalent (4/6), followed by executive functioning and visuoconstruction (3/6) and language and non-verbal memory (2/6). In all patients with 3 affected cognitive domains language was present, and verbal memory and executive functioning in most (2/3). None of the patients had borderline to impaired scores on all three tests on which performance of our group was reduced, i.e. letter fluency test, VTS-S3-RT and Beery VMI, nor was another neuropsychological test profile detected consistent among all 9 patients. In the logistic regression neither age (coefficient = − 0.021, standard error 0.025, p = 0.397) nor the presence of MRI abnormalities (coefficient = − 0.56, standard error = 0.788; p = 0.478) were statistically significant predictors for the presence of borderline T scores across 2 or more cognitive domains. Only one patient (3%) scored in the impaired range (T scores ≤29 across 2 cognitive domains).

Discussion

This study confirms that overall cognitive functioning of adult male ALD patients with a normal MRI or minimal MRI abnormalities seems intact, but that significant individual variability exists in 27.3%. The majority (24.2%) show borderline scores (T-score > 29 ≤ 36; see Table 4) and only 3% show an impairment.

Although overall cognitive functioning was intact, subtle cognitive deficits were detected when comparing the average and the distribution of test scores of our patient group to standardized norm group on visuoconstructive tasks (Beery VMI and RCFT copy subtest; 6/31), mental reaction time measured during a complex decision task (VTS-S3-RT; 5/28) and on a verbal fluency task (letter fluency test; 5/33). Moreover, qualitative case-by-case analyses revealed that 9/33 (27.3%) patients had borderline or impaired performances across 2 or more cognitive domains. However, the distribution of these lower scores were heterogeneous over the cognitive domains and contradictive. For instance a borderline score on a decision psychomotor speed test while another speed and executive tests were normal. Additional follow-up studies, however, are necessary to confirm if this borderline to impaired performance reflects an impaired neuropsychological profile and may represent a risk profile for cerebral X-ALD.

As previous findings in the study of Edwin et al. (1996) were limited [7], this study measured cognitive functions more broadly and used two or more (sub)tests for each cognitive domain (visuoconstruction, executive functioning, psychomotor speed, memory and language). Furthermore, this study used a 3 T MRI that has a higher resolution and can detect smaller lesions than the 1.5 T MRI that was used in the study of Edwin et al. [7]. Our findings support the findings of Edwin et al. (1996) as patients showed subtle cognitive deficits on visuoconstructive functioning [7]. Besides, our study showed a weaker verbal fluency, that was also seen in a previous study on asymptomatic ALD boys [7, 10]. Moreover, Edwin et al. (1996) reported impaired verbal fluency relatively early in the cerebral manifestation of the disease [7]. Likewise, we replicated the deficits within psychomotor speed reported by Edwin et al. (1996) [7], although in our study this deficit was only present on a task measuring mental reaction during a more complex decision. This difference could be caused by the task used, as Edwin et al. (1996) assessed psychomotor speed with the Grooved Pegboard task, which relies highly on fine fingertip dexterity and measures motor speed and we administered the Vienna Test System [7, 31], which makes a distinction in motor and mental reaction time [22]. Perhaps ALD patients have difficulties in decision-making in a more complex situation (e.g. when more stimuli need to be interpreted instead of a single stimulus), but gross motor function of the arm is still intact. Furthermore, as reported by others [7, 8, 10], executive functioning seems intact, although verbal fluency and mental reaction time during a complex decision task were slightly impaired in our cohort, which also highly rely on executive abilities [13, 22].

In some patients borderline to impaired scores are present even in the absence of a significant white matter lesion load on MRI. Hypothetically, functional abnormalities of the white matter caused by mutations in the ABCD1 gene – the underlying genetic defect in ALD - or very early signs of inflammatory demyelinating lesions under the detection limit of MRI might already be present in these patients [32]. Quantitative neuroimaging studies using magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) have shown alterations in metabolite levels in normal appearing white matter of ALD patients [33,34,35]. In addition, as the inflammatory cerebral manifestation of ALD manifests itself mostly in the splenium of the corpus callosum extending into the parieto-occipital white matter, this could reflect the cognitive deficits we found in visuoconstruction and mental reaction time [36, 37]. Less often white matter lesions are observed in the genu of the corpus callosum and progress to frontal white matter [6, 12, 34, 38], which could represent the somewhat affected verbal fluency. Moreover, like the splenium and the parieto-occipital white matter, the frontal brain regions are also involved in mental reaction time [36]. However, due to the small number of patients in this study these speculations need to be confirmed in future studies.

Although this study reports valuable data on the cognitive functioning of adult male ALD patients with no or minor MRI abnormalities, various uncertainties in the interpretations of our results remain. While this study is one of the larger ALD cohort studies, the size of the sample was still small and we had to exclude some test battery elements in some patients. This reduces statistical power, makes proper adjustment for confounders impossible and caution in the interpretation of our results is warranted as this might have caused selection bias and type II errors (not finding impairment when they are actually there) cannot be ruled out. Despite a relatively small sample size, the subgroup analyses do suggest that the sample was representative for other ALD patients. The degree of cognitive dysfunction in ALD patients has been correlated to lesion load on MRI [6, 7, 10, 39], and in our cohort 4 patients had ALD lesions on MRI and 12 minor vascular lesions (maximum Fazekas grade 1). Indeed, vascular lesions are associated with cognitive dysfunction [40]. But, vascular lesions are frequently present in the general population and therefore probably also in participants included in the standardized norm group. Results remained the same when excluding the subgroups with MRI abnormalities. Furthermore, 5/9 patients with borderline to impaired performances across 2 or more cognitive domains had a completely normal MRI. In addition, regression analyses confirmed that the presence of MRI abnormalities was not a significant predictor of the presence of T scores ≤36 within 2 or more cognitive domains. Moreover, although the distribution of educational levels differed from the general Dutch population, test scores were adjusted for education level reducing possible selection bias. Meanwhile, it remains unclear if the diminished RCFT-copy test results reflects clinically relevant information on visuoconstruction, as criterion validity (how well test results are related to a clinical outcome) of this test is marginal [41]. This study used Dutch standardized norm groups (N = 276–1600). The advantage of such large reference groups is the possibility to correct for the influence of age, education level and/or gender. This is not possible in often used smaller case control or control groups.

A major shortcoming of this study is that only cross-sectional data from the cohort is available at this time with individual data at one time point for patients across a wide range of ages. This neglects the temporal nature of X-ALD and the possibility of within individual age-related changes over the life time. In addition, multivariable analyses were not possible using the current methods. Follow-up is needed and is planned in order to monitor cognitive functioning within this cohort and to evaluate if alterations across these cognitive domains precede the onset of the cerebral manifestation of the disease. If the detected abnormalities persist and progress, cognitive functioning can have predictive value superior to currently used structural MRI. Identification of patients with the cerebral manifestation remains important as illustrated by recent work of Pierpont et al. [42]. Even in boys with a relatively low lesion load on MRI (Loes score ≤ 4.5) severe cognitive impairments were detected 4 years after haematopoietic stem cell transplantation [42].

In conclusion, this study shows that cognitive functioning seems intact in adult male ALD patients with no or minimal MRI abnormalities. However, there are indications of borderline scores and cognitive impairments in a subgroup of patients affecting the domains of visuoconstruction, verbal fluency, mental reaction time and possibly executive functioning. The necessity for prospective studies remains to assess the relevance of this deviant scores and if neuropsychological assessment – perhaps in combination with advanced MRI techniques - can detect the onset of cerebral inflammatory demyelination before structural MRI.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Kemp S, Huffnagel IC, Linthorst GE, Wanders RJ, Engelen M. Adrenoleukodystrophy - neuroendocrine pathogenesis and redefinition of natural history. Nat Rev Endocrinol. 2016;12(10):606–15.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Aubourg P, Blanche S, Jambaque I, Rocchiccioli F, Kalifa G, Naud-Saudreau C, et al. Reversal of early neurologic and neuroradiologic manifestations of X-linked adrenoleukodystrophy by bone marrow transplantation. N Engl J Med. 1990;322(26):1860–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Shapiro E, Krivit W, Lockman L, Jambaque I, Peters C, Cowan M, et al. Long-term effect of bone-marrow transplantation for childhood-onset cerebral X-linked adrenoleukodystrophy. Lancet. 2000;356(9231):713–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Miller WP, Rothman SM, Nascene D, Kivisto T, DeFor TE, Ziegler RS, et al. Outcomes after allogeneic hematopoietic cell transplantation for childhood cerebral adrenoleukodystrophy: the largest single-institution cohort report. Blood. 2011;118(7):1971–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Beam D, Poe MD, Provenzale JM, Szabolcs P, Martin PL, Prasad V, et al. Outcomes of unrelated umbilical cord blood transplantation for X-linked adrenoleukodystrophy. Biol Blood Marrow Transplant. 2007;13(6):665–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Engelen M, Kemp S, de Visser M, van Geel BM, Wanders RJ, Aubourg P, et al. X-linked adrenoleukodystrophy (X-ALD): clinical presentation and guidelines for diagnosis, follow-up and management. Orphanet J Rare Dis. 2012;7:51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Edwin D, Speedie LJ, Kohler W, Naidu S, Kruse B, Moser HW. Cognitive and brain magnetic resonance imaging findings in adrenomyeloneuropathy. Ann Neurol. 1996;40(4):675–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Cox CS, Dubey P, Raymond GV, Mahmood A, Moser AB, Moser HW. Cognitive evaluation of neurologically asymptomatic boys with X-linked adrenoleukodystrophy. Arch Neurol. 2006;63(1):69–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Kaga M, Furushima W, Inagaki M, Nakamura M. Early neuropsychological signs of childhood adrenoleukodystrophy (ALD). Brain and Development. 2009;31(7):558–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Riva D, Bova SM, Bruzzone MG. Neuropsychological testing may predict early progression of asymptomatic adrenoleukodystrophy. Neurology. 2000;54(8):1651–5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Huffnagel IC, van de Beek MC, Showers AL, Orsini JJ, Klouwer FCC, Dijkstra IME, et al. Comparison of C26:0-carnitine and C26:0-lysophosphatidylcholine as diagnostic markers in dried blood spots from newborns and patients with adrenoleukodystrophy. Mol Genet Metab. 2017;122(4):209–15.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Loes DJ, Hite S, Moser H, Stillman AE, Shapiro E, Lockman L, et al. Adrenoleukodystrophy: a scoring method for brain MR observations. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1994;15(9):1761–6.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Schmand B, Groenink SC, van den Dungen M. Letter fluency: psychometric properties and Dutch normative data. Tijdschr Gerontol Geriatr. 2008;39(2):64–76.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Wechsler D. WAIS-IV-NL Wechsler adult intelligence scale-IV-NL. Amsterdam: Pearson Assessment & Information BV; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Saan RJ, Deelman BG. De 15-woordentest A en B (een voorlopige handleiding). Groningen: University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Neuropsychology; 1986.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Schmidt M. Rey auditory verbal learning test: a handbook. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Wechsler D, Hendriks HPH, Boumans Z, Kessels RPC, Aldenkamp AP. Wechsler memory scale – fourth edition (WMS-IV), Dutch translation. 4th ed. Amsterdam: Pearson; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Meyers JE, Meyers KR. Rey complex figure test and recognition trial professional manual. Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Beery KE, Beery NA. Administration, scoring, and teaching manual for the Beery-VMI. 6th ed. San Antionio, TX: Pearson; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Reitan RM. Trail making test: manual for administration and scoring. Mesa, Arizona: Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Hammes JGW. The STROOP color-word test: manual. Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Schuhfried G. Vienna test system (VTS) 8 8.2 ed. Austria Moedling; 2013.

  23. Scholte E, Noens I. BRIEF-A. Vragenlijst over executieve functies bij volwassenen. Amsterdam: Hogrefe; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  24. MedCalc Statistical Software Ostend, Belgium: MedCalc Software byba; 2017 [version 17.9:[Available from: https://www.medcalc.org.

  25. Bouma A, Mulder J, Lindeboom J, Schmand BA. Handboek neuropsychologische diagnostiek 2e herziende druk. 2nd ed. Amsterdam: Pearson; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D. Neuropsychological assessment 5th edition. 5th ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Field A. Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. 4th ed. Londen: Sage Publications Ltd; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Verhage F. Educational classification system for research purposes: revised version. Groningen: University Medical Center Groningen; 1983.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Statistiek CBvd. StatLine 2018 [Available from: http://statline.cbs.nl/Statweb/.

  30. Fazekas F, Chawluk JB, Alavi A, Hurtig HI, Zimmerman RA. MR signal abnormalities at 1.5 T in Alzheimer's dementia and normal aging. AJR Am J Roentgenol. 1987;149(2):351–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Prieler J. Vienna Test system reaction test manual. 2008.

  32. Mosser J, Douar AM, Sarde CO, Kioschis P, Feil R, Moser H, et al. Putative X-linked adrenoleukodystrophy gene shares unexpected homology with ABC transporters. Nature. 1993;361(6414):726–30.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Eichler FS, Itoh R, Barker PB, Mori S, Garrett ES, van Zijl PC, et al. Proton MR spectroscopic and diffusion tensor brain MR imaging in X-linked adrenoleukodystrophy: initial experience. Radiology. 2002;225(1):245–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Rajanayagam V, Balthazor M, Shapiro EG, Krivit W, Lockman L, Stillman AE. Proton MR spectroscopy and neuropsychological testing in adrenoleukodystrophy. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1997;18(10):1909–14.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Dubey P, Fatemi A, Huang H, Nagae-Poetscher L, Wakana S, Barker PB, et al. Diffusion tensor-based imaging reveals occult abnormalities in adrenomyeloneuropathy. Ann Neurol. 2005;58(5):758–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Turken A, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Bammer R, Baldo JV, Dronkers NF, Gabrieli JD. Cognitive processing speed and the structure of white matter pathways: convergent evidence from normal variation and lesion studies. Neuroimage. 2008;42(2):1032–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Madden DJ, Whiting WL, Huettel SA, White LE, MacFall JR, Provenzale JM. Diffusion tensor imaging of adult age differences in cerebral white matter: relation to response time. Neuroimage. 2004;21(3):1174–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. McKinney AM, Nascene D, Miller WP, Eisengart J, Loes D, Benson M, et al. Childhood cerebral X-linked adrenoleukodystrophy: diffusion tensor imaging measurements for prediction of clinical outcome after hematopoietic stem cell transplantation. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2013;34(3):641–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Moser HW, Loes DJ, Melhem ER, Raymond GV, Bezman L, Cox CS, et al. X-linked adrenoleukodystrophy: overview and prognosis as a function of age and brain magnetic resonance imaging abnormality. A study involving 372 patients. Neuropediatrics. 2000;31(5):227–39.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Birdsill AC, Koscik RL, Jonaitis EM, Johnson SC, Okonkwo OC, Hermann BP, et al. Regional white matter hyperintensities: aging, Alzheimer's disease risk, and cognitive function. Neurobiol Aging. 2014;35(4):769–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Egberink IJL, Vermeulen CSM. Cotan beoordeling Rey complex figure test. Amsterdam: Nederlands Instituut Psychologen; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Pierpont EI, Eisengart JB, Shanley R, Nascene D, Raymond GV, Shapiro EG, et al. Neurocognitive trajectory of boys who received a hematopoietic stem cell transplant at an early stage of childhood cerebral Adrenoleukodystrophy. JAMA Neurol. 2017;74(6):710–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all men that participated in this study for their participation, effort and time.

Funding

This study was not funded by any kind of specific grant from funding agencies (public, commercial, or not-for-profit).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

NB and SB contributed to acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of data and manuscript preparation. IH contributed to the study concept and design, acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of data and manuscript preparation. ME contributed to the acquisition of data and the critical revision of manuscript for intellectual content. ME, KO and GJ contributed to the study concept and design, analysis and interpretation of data and critical revision of manuscript for intellectual content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gert J. Geurtsen.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study protocol was approved by the local Institutional Review Board (METC 2016_012). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants and is available upon request.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

N.B., S.B., M.S., K.O. and G.J. report no disclosures.

I.H. is consultant to Vertex. M.E. received research grants from Vertex and Minoryx Therapeutics and is consultant to Vertex and Minoryx Therapeutics. None of the disclosures are relevant to the design of this study, nor the manuscript preparation or content.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Buermans, N.J.M.L., van den Bosch, S.J.G., Huffnagel, I.C. et al. Overall intact cognitive function in male X-linked adrenoleukodystrophy adults with normal MRI. Orphanet J Rare Dis 14, 217 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13023-019-1184-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13023-019-1184-4

Keywords