Skip to main content

Event-related potential (ERP) evidence for visual processing differences in children and adults with cystinosis (CTNS gene mutations)

Abstract

Background

Cystinosis, a rare lysosomal storage disease caused by mutations in the CTNS gene, is characterized by cystine crystallization and accumulation within multiple tissues, including kidney and brain. Its impact on neural function appears mild relative to its effects on other organs during early disease, but since therapeutic advances have led to substantially increased life expectancy, neurological implications are of increasing interest, necessitating deeper understanding of the impact of cystinosis on neurocognitive function. Behavioral difficulties have been reported in cystinosis in the visual domain. Very little is known, however, about how the brains of people living with cystinosis process visual information. This is especially interesting given that cystine accumulation in the cornea and posterior ocular structures is a hallmark of cystinosis.

Methods

Here, high-density scalp electrophysiology was recorded to visual stimuli (during a Go/No-Go task) to investigate visual processing in individuals with cystinosis, compared to age-matched controls. Analyses focused on early stages of cortical visual processing.

Results

The groups differed in their initial cortical response, with individuals with cystinosis exhibiting a significantly larger visual evoked potential (VEP) in the 130–150 ms time window. The groups also differed in the associations between neural responses and verbal abilities: While controls with higher IQ scores presented larger neural responses, that relationship was not observed in cystinosis.

Conclusions

The enlarged VEP in cystinosis could be the result of cortical hyperexcitability and/or differences in attentional engagement and explain, at least partially, the visual and visual-spatial difficulties described in this population.

Introduction

Cystinosis is an autosomal recessive disorder caused by bi-allelic mutations in the 17p13.2-located CTNS gene [1] with an incidence rate of around one in 100,000 to 200,000 live births [2, 3]. The accumulation of cystine in cells [4, 5], characteristic of the condition, leads to deregulation of endocytosis and cell signaling [6]. Ultimately, intralysosomal cystine crystallizes, triggering significant damage in a multitude of tissues and organs [7]. Renal, retinal, endocrinological, muscular, and neurological complications are observed [8, 9]. Due to advances in drug development and the availability of renal replacement therapy [10, 11], the life expectancy of individuals with cystinosis has, however, increased well into adulthood, demanding a better understanding of the developmental trajectories associated with the condition.

To reduce this gap in knowledge, our research team has focused on characterizing the impact of cystinosis on brain and cognitive function. Though abnormally high levels of cystine have been observed in different brain regions [12,13,14], cystinosis’ impact on brain activity is still not well understood. In previous work focusing on auditory processing and sensory memory, we reported generally maintained sensory processing, but some differences in sensory memory in children and adults living with cystinosis [15, 16].

Here, we expand this investigation to the visual sensory domain. Interestingly, visual-perceptual indices are often significantly lower than verbal indices in individuals with cystinosis [17,18,19]. This pattern emerges early in development and persists throughout the lifespan [20, 21], regardless of age at treatment onset [22]. Significant difficulties have also been described in domains related to visual-motor, visual-spatial and visual memory skills [22,23,24,25,26], but see [27] for a report of maintained visual learning in adults with cystinosis. Despite this pattern of relative weaknesses in visual processing domains, very little is known about how the brains of people living with cystinosis process visual information. One case study tested visual processing in two children with cystinosis before and after kidney transplantation. During dialysis treatment, both children showed delayed and decreased visual evoked responses, but typical brain responses were seen after kidney transplantation [28]. Despite the very small number of individuals tested to date, EEG measurements appear to be sensitive to neuropathology in cystinosis and could be useful as outcome measures to assess the impact of treatment on brain function.

Here, we use high-density EEG to investigate basic visual processing in a group of individuals with cystinosis and compare them to a group of age-matched controls. The analyses focus on the visual evoked potential (VEP) component P1. P1 is an early VEP peaking around 100 ms following stimulus onset and has been associated with multiple generators in both dorsal and ventral visual streams [29,30,31,32,33]. Additionally, we tested for associations between neural responses and age and standardized cognitive measures.

Considering the behavioral difficulties reported in visual-related processing in cystinosis and the tendency for cystine accumulation in the retina, we hypothesized that individuals with cystinosis would show different sensory-perceptual brain responses to visually presented stimuli, reflected in amplitude differences in the P1 component of the VEP. Identifying the processing stages that are impaired and contribute to visual and visual-spatial processing differences in cystinosis will be important in the development of impactful strategies that address visual processing difficulties and identify sensitive biomarkers of treatment efficacy on brain function.

Materials and methods

Participants

Thirty-eight individuals diagnosed with cystinosis (CYS; age range: 7–36 years old, 25 women) and 45 age-matched controls (CT; 27 women) were recruited. Individuals with cystinosis were recruited via social media and through contact with family organizations. Due to the rareness of cystinosis, most participants, all of whom lived within the United States, traveled from out-of-state to participate. Furthermore, the controls were recruited via flyers in the neighborhoods surrounding the lab and through a lab-maintained participant database. Developmental and/or educational difficulties or delays, neurological problems, and a severe mental illness diagnosis were exclusionary criteria for controls. Current neurological problems were exclusionary criteria for individuals living with cystinosis. To assess visual acuity, a Snellen chart was used. All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision. Nevertheless, all participants were asked at the start of the electrophysiological experimental session if they could see the stimuli and their different components without difficulty. One individual with cystinosis was excluded from the final sample due to illness on the scheduled day of testing. All individuals, and their legal guardian if under 18 years old, signed a consent form. Participants were monetarily compensated for their time. This study and all the associated procedures were approved by the Albert Einstein College of Medicine Institutional Review Board (IRB 2009-523). All aspects of the research conformed to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Experimental procedure and stimuli

Participation consisted of two visits, which involved completion of a cognitive function battery and EEG recordings. The cognitive function battery included the assessment of verbal and non-verbal intelligence (using age-appropriate Wechsler Intelligence Scales) and executive functioning components (Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System, D-KEFS; [34] and the Conners Continuous Performance Test 3, CPT; [35]). During the EEG recording session, participants were asked to respond to different tasks assessing sensory processing and response inhibition. Here, we focus on basic visual processing of images presented in the context of a Go/No-Go task. Response inhibition (cognitive function and EEG) findings are reported separately [36].

During the EEG Go/No-Go task, positive and neutral valence images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) were presented in a pseudorandom sequence. Participants were instructed to press the left mouse button with the right index finger upon each stimulus presentation, as quickly and as accurately as possible, unless the stimulus was a repetition of the immediately preceding stimulus, in which case they should withhold their response (i.e., not push the mouse button). Stimuli, subtended 8.6° horizontally by 6.5° vertically, were presented centrally for 600 ms at an average rate of 1 per second (every 950–1050 ms with a random temporal jitter within this 100 ms window). Three 12-min blocks were run. Each block consisted of 540 trials, for a total of 1620 per participant. Here, we focus exclusively on hit trials, that is, trials that included a correct button press after stimulus presentation (only hits preceded by another hit were included), to ensure a good signal-to-noise ratio and to avoid overlap of processes related to inhibiting a response (on withhold trials).

Data acquisition and analysis

Continuous EEG data were recorded from 64 scalp electrodes at a sampling rate of 512 Hz (Active 2 system; Biosemi™, The Netherlands; 10–20 montage) and preprocessed using the EEGLAB toolbox (version 2021.0) [37] for MATLAB (version 2021a; MathWorks, Natick, MA) (the full pipeline can be accessed at: https://github.com/DouweHorsthuis) [38]. Preprocessing steps included down-sampling data to 256 Hz, re-referencing to the average, and filtering with a 0.1 Hz high pass filter (0.1 Hz transition bandwidth, filter order 16,896) and a 45 Hz low pass filter (11 Hz transition bandwidth, filter order 152). Both were zero-phase Hamming windowed sinc FIR filters. Noisy channels were excluded based on kurtosis and visual confirmation. Artifacts from blinks and saccades were eliminated via Independent Component Analysis (ICA). The spherical spline method was then used to interpolate channels that were removed in previous steps. Data were segmented into epochs of −100 ms to 400 ms using a baseline of −100 ms to 0 ms. These epochs went through an artifact detection algorithm (moving window peak-to-peak threshold at 120 µV). To equate number of epochs in each participants averaged VEP, 200 epochs meeting the criteria for hits were randomly selected per participant.

P1 was measured between 130 and 150 ms at O1, Oz, and O2. Time windows and electrode locations were selected based on past research and confirmed (and adjusted) by inspecting the timing and topography of the major voltage fluctuations in the grand averages. Mean amplitude data were used for both between-groups statistics and Spearman correlations. All p-values (from t-tests and Spearman correlations) were submitted to Holm-Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons [39], using the p.adjust of the stats package in R [40]. Mixed-effects models were implemented to analyze trial-by-trial data, using the lmer function in the lme4 package [41] in R [40]. Group was always a fixed factor. Participants and trials were added as random factors. Models were fit using the maximum likelihood criterion. p-values were estimated using Satterthwaite approximations.

Results

Demographics and cognitive function measures

Table 1 shows a summary of the included participants’ age, sex, and cognitive functioning (verbal and non-verbal IQ). Two-sample independent-means t-tests were run in R [40] to test for group differences in age and cognitive performance. In cases in which the assumption of the homogeneity of variances was violated, Welch corrections were applied to adjust the degrees of freedom. A chi-square test was run to test for independence between sex and group. Effect sizes were calculated utilizing Cohen’s d and w. As can be seen in Table 1 and Fig. 1, the groups differed in verbal and non-verbal abilities, with individuals with cystinosis showing more difficulties than their age-matched peers. No differences were found in age and sex between the groups.

Table 1 Characterization of the control and cystinosis individuals included in the analyses: demographics and cognitive function (IQ and inhibition measures)
Fig. 1
figure 1

Included participants’ verbal and non-verbal IQ scores. Violin plots showing individual data-points (orange and pink) and median values (black dots)

Basic visual processing: P1

The averaged VEPs for P1 per channel and by group can be seen in Fig. 2. Mixed-effects models were implemented as described in the Methods Section. Average amplitude at O1-Oz-O2 at each trial was the numeric dependent variable. As can be appreciated in the waveforms and scalp topographic maps in Fig. 2, individuals with cystinosis showed significantly increased P1 amplitudes compared to their age-matched peers (ß = 7.55, SE = 2.45, p = 0.01).

Fig. 2
figure 2

A Averaged ERPs per group and channel (O1, Oz, and O2). Shaded areas indicate window of interest. Asterisks indicate significant differences. B Topographical maps showing brain activity in the P1 time window per group. C Violin plots showing distribution of single trial data amplitudes per group at O1, Oz, and O2 (average) for P1. Small dots indicate amplitude at a given trial and central dots group mean amplitude. Asterisks indicate significant differences

Correlations

Figure 3 shows neural response correlations with age (Panel A), verbal IQ (Panel B), and non-verbal IQ (Panel C). P1significantly correlated with age in both groups, with amplitudes decreasing with age (rs = −0.73, p = 0.01). As can be seen in Fig. 3 Panel B, the groups differed in the correlations between P1 and verbal IQ. While controls showed a positive correlation between P1 and verbal IQ (rs = 0.40, p = 0.04), such correlation was not significant in cystinosis. P1 did not correlate significantly with non-verbal IQ, for either the control or the cystinosis group.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Spearman correlations between P1 and age (A), verbal IQ (B), and non-verbal IQ (C). Correlation coefficients and their significance are presented for the full sample when groups did not differ (age and non-verbal IQ) and per group when groups differed (verbal IQ)

Discussion

Despite ample evidence for atypical visual processing in cystinosis in the neuropsychological literature [20, 26, 42] and clinical reports of hyper-sensitivity to visual stimulation, the integrity of basic visual sensory processing in this group has not been extensively explored. To begin to address this knowledge gap, we used scalp recordings of electrophysiological responses to visual stimuli to compare the amplitude of the visual P1, the earliest major neurophysiological response that was detectable in our data, between a group of individuals with cystinosis and an age matched control group. This revealed that in the individuals with cystinosis, the amplitude of the P1 was markedly amplified.

Increased sensory-evoked potentials may reflect increased excitability in sensory cortices [43,44,45,46,47]. An increased P1 in cystinosis could thus signify visual cortex hyperexcitability in this population. Visual cortex hyperexcitability has been linked to photophobia in individuals suffering from migraines [43], who may also present with increased visual evoked potentials (such as the P1)—though not consistently so (see [44] for a summary of different results). Importantly, photophobia is the most frequently reported ocular symptom in cystinosis [3, 48, 49]. Though we did not quantify photophobia in our cystinosis sample, comments regarding general sensitivity to light were common during testing. The pathophysiology of photophobia in relation to crystal accumulation in cystinosis is unclear. Our finding of a significantly increased visual evoked response in cystinosis opens the possibility that hypersensitivity to light could be related to altered visual processing due to visual cortex hyperexcitability.

Although we think it unlikely, one cannot rule out the possibility that group differences in P1 amplitude, alternatively, reflect different levels of attentional engagement. Selective attention, most often in the context of visuo-spatial attention designs, has been shown to significantly modulate P1 amplitude [50,51,52,53,54,55,56]. Such modulation may reflect a sensory gain type mechanism that results in enhanced perceptual processing of attended stimuli [57]. However, it is important to point out that attentional modulations of the P1 are typically only reported for stimuli that are presented peripherally (i.e. inputs that are not foveated) during selective covert spatial attention tasks, and a number of studies have shown that experimental manipulations of attention in the context of foveally presented stimuli do not induce P1 modulations (e.g., [58, 59]) or that it requires special manipulation of the nature of the centrally presented inputs [60]. Interestingly, we have previously reported increased P2 and P3a amplitudes in a sample of adults with cystinosis in the context of a passive auditory duration oddball stimulation [15]. We argued then that individuals with cystinosis may engage attention differently, which the current findings could be further evidence of. Studies focused on attentional processes in this population are needed, particularly those that relate to potential differences in attention engagement.

P1 has been localized to sources in the dorsal extrastriate cortex, specifically to areas V3, V3a, and adjacent middle occipital gyrus and in the ventral extrastriate cortex, specifically area V4 in the fusiform gyrus [29,30,31,32] and it is thus driven by both magnocellular and parvocellular input [61]. Traditionally, V3 and V3a have been associated with motion processing (e.g., [62]). However, studies in non-human primates suggest that these areas may also be involved in linking higher-level parietal and temporal processing streams [63] and may play a role in the integration of visual stimulus features, essential for global stimulus processing [64]. V4 has been associated with the processing of surface properties (color, brightness, texture), shape (orientation, curvature), motion and motion contrast, and depth; but it has also been shown to play a crucial role in visual attention [65]. Moreover, in non-human primates, V4 appears to be widely interconnected with other visual areas along the ventral and dorsal visual streams, frontal areas, and subcortical structures, and thus has been conceived as holding an integrative role in visual perception and recognition and, potentially, in guiding perceptual decisions and higher-order behavior [66]. Structural and/or functional neuroimaging studies examining V3, V3a, and V4 could be particularly informative in understanding the mechanistic roles that these areas play in the visual-spatial and visual attention differences described in cystinosis.

The increased amplitude VEP in cystinosis appears to be especially localized to the occipital and lateral parietal-occipital channels. While the current analyses focused on the channels in which response was maximal for both groups, those channels also represented the largest differences between the groups. As one can appreciate in Additional file 1: Figure S1, differences between the groups are reduced in lateral parietal-occipital channels and absent in mid-line parietal-occipital and across parietal channels. Future studies directed at defining the underlying neural sources explaining these differences are justified.

Correlational analyses revealed that neural indices of visual sensory processing correlated with age in a similar fashion across groups. In the general population, VEPs attenuate in amplitude through at least late adolescence (Brandwein et al. 2011). Here, P1 attenuation was replicated in our control sample and was also observed in the cystinosis group. The groups differed, however, in the correlations between P1 and verbal IQ. While controls with higher IQ scores showed larger P1s, a significant relationship between P1 and verbal IQ was not present in the cystinosis group. P1 did not correlate significantly with non-verbal IQ, for either the control or the cystinosis group.

Lastly, a brief detailing of findings relating to general cognitive function in cystinosis is merited. Neurocognitive assessments showed lower verbal and non-verbal IQs in individuals with cystinosis, when compared to their control peers. We and others had previously reported lower IQ scores in this population [15, 16, 19, 23] and, as in other studies, our findings (see Table 1) indicate greater difficulties in non-verbal processing [18,19,20,21,22, 27]. Of note, scales like the Wechsler Scales of Intelligence have an unbalanced number of timed non-verbal vs verbal subtests. To more accurately characterize the cognitive profile associated with cystinosis, it would be important to investigate whether, given the time, individuals with cystinosis would still show marked difficulties in non-verbal tasks or whether such difficulties are mainly explained by processing speed differences. While this would complicate comparisons with normative data, it could be useful to understand discrepancies between verbal versus non-verbal subtests.

This study is not without limitations. First, our groups were not matched in terms of IQ, with the cystinosis group presenting significantly lower scores than the control group. Although there is no evidence of the association between VEPs and IQ, such differences could have impacted the results. Second, variables related to current health status (such as a measure of renal function) and compliance with treatment, which has been linked to better clinical outcomes [67], were not included in the present study but could be useful in understanding group- and individual-level differences. Lastly, most of the individuals with cystinosis, and differently from those included in the control group, travelled to the lab from out-of-state and completed the study across two consecutive days, which could have increased tiredness in this group.

To conclude, here we present evidence of visual processing differences in cystinosis which could contribute, at least partially, to both photosensitivity and to the behavioral visual-spatial difficulties described in this population. More work is needed to describe how visual processing differences might contribute to the cognitive profile associated with cystinosis. A better understanding of such associations could contribute to the identification of sensitive biomarkers of treatment efficacy on brain function. The current findings should motivate future studies utilizing paradigms tapping into higher- and lower-order visual processes and investigating visual processing and pathways in more depth, for instance, by employing tasks distinguishing between ventral and dorsal streams and magnocellular and parvocellular pathways. However, in so doing we believe it is important for future EEG research with this population to take sensitivity to light into consideration when designing visual experiments. Furthermore, these findings and their implications in classroom and professional settings utilizing digital media and screens should be further explored.

Availability of data and materials

All data are available from the corresponding authors on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

EEG:

Electrophysiological

VEP:

Visual-evoked potential

CT:

Controls

CYS:

Cystinosis

References

  1. Town M, Jean G, Cherqui S, Attard M, Forestier L, Whitmore SA, et al. A novel gene encoding an integral membrane protein is mutated in nephropathic cystinosis. J Nat Genet. 1998;18(4):319–24.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Gahl WA, Thoene JG, Schneider JA. Cystinosis. N Engl J Med. 2009;347(2):111–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Gahl WA, Thoene JG, Schneider JA. Cystinosis. N Engl J Med. 2002;347(2):111–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Gahl WA, Bashan N, Tietze F, Bernardini I, Schulman JD. Cystine transport is defective in isolated leukocyte lysosomes from patients with cystinosis. Science. 1982;217(4566):1263–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Jonas AJ, Greene AA, Smith ML, Schneider JA. Cystine accumulation and loss in normal, heterozygous, and cystinotic fibroblasts. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1982;79(14):4442–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Ivanova E, De Leo MG, De Matteis MA, Levtchenko E. Cystinosis: clinical presentation, pathogenesis and treatment. Pediatric Endocrinol Rev. 2014;12:176–84.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Gahl WA, Kaiser-Kupfer MI. Complications of nephropathic cystinosis after renal failure. Pediatr Nephrol. 1987;1:260–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Elmonem MA, Veys KR, Soliman NA, van Dyck M, van den Heuvel L, Levtchenko E. Cystinosis: a review. Orphanet J Rare Dis. 2016;11(47):1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Bäumner S, Weber LT. Nephropathic Cystinosis: symptoms, treatment, and perspectives of a systemic disease. Front Pediatr. 2018;6:58.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Ariceta G, Giordano V, Santos F. Effects of long-term cysteamine treatment in patients with cystinosis. Pediatr Nephrol. 2019;34(4):571–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Langman CB, Barshop BA, Deschênes G, Emma F, Goodyer P, Lipkin G, et al. Controversies and research agenda in nephropathic cystinosis: conclusions from a “Kidney Disease: Improving Global Outcomes”(KDIGO) Controversies Conference. Kidney Int. 2016;89(6):1192–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Levine S, Paparo G. Brain lesions in a case of cystinosis. Acta Neuropathol. 1982;57(2):217–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Theodoropoulos DS, Krasnewich D, Kaiser-Kupfer MI, Gahl WA. Classic nephropathic cystinosis as an adult disease. JAMA. 1993;270(18):2200–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Vogel DG, Malekzadeh MH, Cornford ME, Schneider JA, Shields WD, Vinters HV. Central nervous system involvement in nephropathic cystinosis. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 1990;49(6):591–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Francisco AA, Berruti AS, Kaskel FJ, Foxe JJ, Molholm S. Assessing the integrity of auditory processing and sensory memory in adults with cystinosis (CTNS gene mutations). Orphanet J Rare Dis. 2021;16(1):1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Francisco AA, Foxe JJ, Horsthuis DJ, Molholm S. Impaired auditory sensory memory in Cystinosis despite typical sensory processing: a high-density electrical mapping study of the mismatch negativity (MMN). NeuroImage Clin. 2020;25:102170.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Charnas LR, Luciano CA, Dalakas M, Gilliatt RW, Bernardini I, Ishak K, et al. Distal vacuolar myopathy in nephropathic cystinosis. Ann Neurol. 1994;35(2):181–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Spilkin AM, Ballantyne AO, Babchuck LR, Trauner DA. Non-verbal deficits in young children with a genetic metabolic disorder: WPPSI-III performance in Cystinosis. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007;144(4):444–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Ulmer FF, Landolt MA, Ha Vinh R, Huisman TAGM, Neuhaus TJ, Latal B, et al. Intellectual and motor performance, quality of life and psychosocial adjustment in children with cystinosis. Pediatr Nephrol. 2009;24:1371–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Scarvie KM, Ballantyne A, Trauner DA. Visuomotor performance in children with infantile nephropathic Cystinosis. Percept Mot Skills. 1996;82:67–75.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Trauner DA, Chase C, Scheller J, Katz B, Schneider JA. Neurologic and cognitive deficits in children with Cystinosis. J Pediatr. 1988;112(6):912–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Viltz L, Trauner DA. Effect of age at treatment on cognitive performance in patients with Cystinosis. J Pediatr. 2013;163:489–92.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Aly R, Makar S, El Bakri A, Soliman NA. Neurocognitive functions and behavioral profiles in children with nephropathic cystinosis. Saudi J Kidney Dis Transpl. 2014;25(6):1224–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Ballantyne A, Spilkin AM, Trauner DA. Executive function in nephropathic cystinosis. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2013;26(1):14–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Besouw MTP, Hulstijn-Dirkmaat GM, van der Rijken REA, Cornelissen EAM, van Dael CM, Vande Walle J, et al. Neurocognitive functioning in school-aged cystinosis patients. J Inherit Metab Dis. 2010;33:787–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Sathappan A, Trauner DA. Hierarchical processing of visual stimuli in nephropathic Cystinosis. J Inherit Metab Dis. 2019;42(3):545–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Frankel AM, Trauner DA. Visual and verbal learning and memory in cystinosis. Brain Cogn. 2019;135: 103578.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Ethier A, Lippé S, Mérouani A, Lassonde M, Saint-Amour D. Reversible visual evoked potential abnormalities in uremic children. Pediatr Neurol. 2012;46(6):390–2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Di Russo F, Martínez A, Sereno MI, Pitzalis S, Hillyard SA. Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;15(2):95–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Foxe JJ, Murray MM, Javitt DC. Filling-in in schizophrenia: a high-density electrical mapping and source-analysis investigation of illusory contour processing. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(12):1914–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Butler PD, Martinez A, Foxe JJ, Kim D, Zemon V, Silipo G, et al. Subcortical visual dysfunction in schizophrenia drives secondary cortical impairments. Brain. 2007;130(2):417–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Yeap S, Kelly SP, Sehatpour P, Magno E, Javitt DC, Garavan H, et al. Early visual sensory deficits as endophenotypes for schizophrenia: high-density electrical mapping in clinically unaffected first-degree relatives. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(11):1180–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Foxe JJ, Simpson GV. Flow of activation from V1 to frontal cortex in humans. A framework for defining “early” visual processing. Exp Brain Res. 2002;142(1):139–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer JH. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: Technical Manual. San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment Company; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Conners CK. Conners’ Rating Scales-Revised technical manual, instruments for use with children and adolescents. New York: Multi-Health Systems; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Francisco AA, Foxe JJ, Berruti A, Horsthuis DJ, Molholm S. Response inhibition and error-monitoring in cystinosis (CTNS gene mutations): behavioral and electrophysiological evidence of a diverse set of difficulties. bioRxiv. 2023.

  37. Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 2004;134(1):9–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Horsthuis DJ, Molholm S, Foxe JJ, Francisco AA. EEG_to_ERP_pipeline_stats_R. Version 2.02 ed2021.

  39. Holm S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand J Stat. 1979;6(2):65–70.

    Google Scholar 

  40. RCoreTeam. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. 3.3.2 ed. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014.

  41. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker BM, Walker SC. Ime4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4 (R package Version 1.1-7) [Computer software]. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/index.html. 2023.

  42. Nichols SL, Press GA, Schneider JA, Trauner DA. Cortical atrophy and cognitive performance in infantile nephropathic cystinosis. Pediatr Neurol. 1990;6(6):379–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Denuelle M, Boulloche N, Payoux PMDP, Fabre N, Trotter Y, Géraud G. A PET study of photophobia during spontaneous migraine attacks. Neurology. 2011;76(3):213–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Sand T, Zhitniy N, White LR, Stovner LJ. Visual evoked potential latency, amplitude and habituation in migraine: a longitudinal study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2008;119(5):1020–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Aurora SK, Cao Y, Bowyer SM, Welch KMA. The occipital cortex is hyperexcitable in migraine: experimental evidence. Headache. 1999;39(7):469–76.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Mulleners WM, Chronicle EP, Palmer JE, Koehler PJ, Vredeveld JW. Visual cortex excitability in migraine with and without aura. Headache. 2001;41(6):565–72.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Coppola G, Schoenen J. Cortical excitability in chronic migraine. Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2012;16(1):93–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Pinxten AM, Hua MT, Simpson J, Hohenfellner K, Levtchenko E, Casteels I. Clinical practice: a proposed standardized ophthalmological assessment for patients with cystinosis. Ophthalmol Ther. 2017;6(1):93–104.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Liang H, Baudouin C, Hassani RTJ, Brignole-Baudouin F, Labbe A. Photophobia and corneal crystal density in nephropathic cystinosis: an in vivo confocal microscopy and anterior-segment optical coherence tomography study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2015;56(5):3218–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Hillyard SA, Anllo-Vento L. Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1998;95(3):781–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Clark VP, Hillyard SA. Spatial selective attention affects early extrastriate but not striate components of the visual evoked potential. J Cogn Neurosci. 1996;8:387–402.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Taylor MJ. Non-spatial attentional effects on P1. Clin Neurophysiol. 2002;113(12):1903–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Fu S, Fedota JR, Greenwood PM, Parasuraman R. Dissociation of visual C1 and P1 components as a function of attentional load: an event-related potential study. Biol Psychol. 2010;85(1):171–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Schroeder CE, Mehta AD, Foxe JJ. Determinants and mechanisms of attentional modulation of neural processing. Front Biosci. 2001;6:672–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Frey HP, Schmid AM, Murphy JW, Molholm S, Lalor EC, Foxe JJ. Modulation of early cortical processing during divided attention to non-contiguous locations. Eur J Neurosci. 2014;39(9):1499–507.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Kelly SP, Gomez-Ramirez M, Foxe JJ. Spatial attention modulates initial afferent activity in human primary visual cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(11):2629–36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Mangun GR, Hillyard SA. Allocation of visual attention to spatial locations: tradeoff functions for event-related brain potentials and detection performance. Percept Psychophys. 1990;47(6):532–50.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Eimer M. An ERP study of sustained spatial attention to stimulus eccentricity. Biol Psychol. 2000;52(3):205–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Handy TC, Khoe W. Attention and sensory gain control: a peripheral visual process? J Cogn Neurosci. 2005;17(12):1936–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Frey HP, Kelly SP, Lalor EC, Foxe JJ. Early spatial attentional modulation of inputs to the fovea. J Neurosci. 2010;30(13):4547–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Martinez A, Di Russo F, Anllo-Vento L, Hillyard SA. Electrophysiological analysis of cortical mechanisms of selective attention to high and low spatial frequencies. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001;112(11):1980–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Felleman DJ, Burkhalter A, Van Essen DC. Cortical connections of areas V3 and VP of macaque monkey extrastriate visual cortex. J Comp Neurol. 1997;379(1):21–47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Arcaro MJ, Kastner S. Topographic organization of areas V3 and V4 and its relation to supra-areal organization of the primate visual system. Vis Neurosci. 2015;32: e014.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Johnson SC, Lowery N, Kohler C, Turetsky BI. Global–local visual processing in schizophrenia: evidence for an early visual processing deficit. Biol Psychiat. 2005;58(12):937–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Roe AW, Chelazzi L, Connor CE, Conway BR, Fujita I, Gallant JL, et al. Toward a unified theory of visual area V4. Neuron. 2012;74(1):12–29.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Pasupathy A, Popovkina DV, Kim T. Visual functions of primate area V4. Annu Rev Vis Sci. 2020;6:363–85.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Brodin-Sartorius A, Tête MJ, Niaudet P, Antignac C, Guest G, Ottolenghi C, et al. Cysteamine therapy delays the progression of nephropathic cystinosis in late adolescents and adults. Kidney Int. 2012;81(2):179–89.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Dr. Juliana Bates, who performed the clinical assessments, Elise Taverna, Danielle Newbury, and Alaina Berruti for their help with data collection, Dr. Frederick J. Kaskel for his help with recruitment, and Hayley Chang for comments on a draft of the manuscript. We extend our most sincere gratitude to the participants and their families for their interest, their involvement, and their time.

Funding

Our work in cystinosis is supported by grants from the Cystinosis Research Network and from the Cystinosis Research Foundation. Additional support for this work is provided by a Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development U54 Grant (HD090260—to SM) to the Human Clinical Phenotyping Core of the Rose F. Kennedy Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center. Work at the collaborating site at the University of Rochester Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center (UR-IDDRC) is supported by a center grant from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (P50 HD103536—to JJF).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AAF, SM, and JJF conceived the study. DJH and AAF collected and analyzed the data. DJH and AAF wrote the first draft of the manuscript. AAF, SM, and JJF provided editorial input to DJH on the subsequent drafts.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Sophie Molholm or Ana A. Francisco.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All participants signed an informed consent approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Albert Einstein College of Medicine. All aspects of the research conformed to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The Authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Averaged ERPs per group over parietal, parietal-occipital, and occipital channels.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Horsthuis, D.J., Molholm, S., Foxe, J.J. et al. Event-related potential (ERP) evidence for visual processing differences in children and adults with cystinosis (CTNS gene mutations). Orphanet J Rare Dis 18, 389 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13023-023-02985-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13023-023-02985-y

Keywords